Working Memory

Working memory is the brain system that temporarily stores and processes information used in complex cognitive functions such as learning, reasoning, language comprehension, long-term memory retrieval, information filtering, and problem solving. It is sometimes conflated with short-term memory because it is temporary in nature and limited in capacity. The difference is that short-term memory is concerned with the passive storage and retrieval of the recent past, while working memory deals with the active processing of the same information. For example, short-term memory allows an individual to remember a message he or she just heard, while working memory allows the person to understand the meaning of the message, determine the relevant information, and formulate a response.

Researchers study working memory to gain a better understanding of how people learn, reason, understand, focus, and solve problems. Such work has contributed to advances in medical knowledge regarding conditions such as Alzheimer's disease and learning disabilities.

OVERVIEW

Stanford University researchers Richard C. Atkinson and Richard M. Shiffrin proposed a model of short-term memory in 1968. In their model, short-term memory holds a limited amount of information for a brief period of time with minimal processing. The model is described as multistore or dual-store because it stores all information, whether visual, spatial, or auditory, via the same system. It is also considered a unitary system, or a single system without any subsystems. This model generated a lot of research, which in turn exposed the flaws in the model.

In 1974, Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch devised an alternative to the Atkinson-Shiffrin model. Called working memory, their model supposes that there are different stores for different types of information, rather than a single store for everything. Baddeley and Hitch also proposed that working memory has a central executive that manages two subsystems. These two subsystems are the phonological loop, which stores auditory information, and the visual-spatial “sketch pad”—also sometimes referred to as the “mind's eye”—which stores visual and spatial information.




Richard C. Atkinson, whose research had an impact on advances in computer-assisted instruction and methods for optimizing the learning process.





Richard C. Atkinson, whose research had an impact on advances in computer-assisted instruction and methods for optimizing the learning process.

Various studies suggest different functions of certain brain regions that contribute to working memory. Experiments dating back to the 1930s identified the prefrontal cortex as a key part of the brain contributing to spatial working memory, and later use of neuroimaging confirmed its importance. However, advanced brain imaging has also shown that working memory involves areas other than the prefrontal cortex, and that most tasks of working memory may in fact use a complex network of different areas. Other areas seen to be activated during certain working memory tasks include the posterior parietal cortex, the thalamus, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the superior frontal sulcus.

People use both visual-spatial and phonological working memory subsystems every day. Together, these subsystems form the basis of the brain's executive function, allowing an individual to pay attention, organize, solve problems, and plan ahead—all of which are key to being able to learn in the classroom, work, and complete numerous other tasks. Both types of working memory usually develop at about the same rate in childhood and reach their peak in early adulthood. The fact that working memory develops over time means that, for example, as students grow older, they are able to follow longer sets of instructions.

Ongoing research has revealed significant factors that may impair working memory, leading to insights into the functional pathways of memory and potential avenues of prevention or treatment of certain disorders. A 2009 study found that strong psychological stress can lower the functionality of the prefrontal cortex, inhibiting working memory. A 2011 paper found that the frequent abuse of alcohol damaged working memory and associated visuospatial capabilities. Conditions such as developmental coordination disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and autism spectrum disorders have been investigated in regards to the influence of working memory.

People with a weakened working memory are more likely to have learning disorders, and working memory has been shown to be a more dependable indicator of a subject's ability to learn than his or her intelligence quotient (IQ). Research has linked poor working memory to a greater likelihood of risk taking, which in turn has been connected to various social problems; a 2015 study found that adolescents with weaker working memories were more likely to become sexually active earlier and engage in unprotected sex. Researchers have also found that because visual-spatial working memory and phonological working memory can operate independently of each other, a person's working memory might be impaired in one area but not in the other. Thus, students with weak visual-spatial working memory might learn to compensate with their stronger auditory working memory and effectively conceal their disability. Testing both kinds of working memory is a critical step toward helping students with learning disabilities devise successful learning plans.

—Lisa U. Phillips, MFA

Atkinson, R. C., and R. M. Shiffrin. “Human Memory: A Proposed System and Its Control Processes.” The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Ed. Kenneth W. Spence and Janet Taylor Spence. Vol. 2. New York: Academic, 1968. 89–195. Print.

Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham Hitch. “Working Memory.” The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Ed. Gordon H. Bower. Vol. 8. New York: Academic, 1974. 47–89. Print.

Bernecker, Sven. Memory: A Philosophical Study. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2010. Print.

Hendrix, Dannie M., and Orval Holcomb, eds. Psychology of Memory. New York: Nova, 2012. Print.

Khurana, Atika, et al. “Stronger Working Memory Reduces Sexual Risk Taking in Adolescents, Even After Controlling for Parental Influences.” Child Development (2015). n. pag. Web. 24 Jun. 2015.

Levin, Eden S., ed. Working Memory: Capacity, Developments and Improvement Techniques. New York: Nova, 2011. Print.

Morin, Amanda. “5 Ways Kids Use Working Memory to Learn.” Understood. Understood.org , 16 Dec. 2013. Web. 24 Jun. 2015.

Sanders, Laura. “Brain Separates Working Memory: Hemispheres Independent in Mental Version of RAM.” Science News 30 July 2011: 10–11. Print.

St. Clair-Thompson, Helen, ed. Working Memory: Developmental Differences, Component Processes and Improvement Mechanisms. New York: Nova, 2013. Print.

Surprenant, Aimée M., and Ian Neath. Principles of Memory. New York: Taylor, 2009. Print.

Weisberg, Robert W., and Lauretta M. Reeves. Cognition: From Memory to Creativity. Hoboken: Wiley, 2013. Print.